womenâs prison in the 1930s, childrenâs preferences seemed to be informed by whoever was feeding them: âBabies who refused tomato juice were found to be fed by adults who also expressed a dislike for tomato juice.âIn a study of preschoolers, a âtargetâ child who preferred one vegetable to another was seated with three classmates who had the opposite preference.By the second day of the study, the target child had already switched preferences. Exposure to people, as much as food itself, influences our liking.
â
Mysteries still abound in our liking for food. Consider the simple question of why we should suddenly like something that we previously disliked. Very few of us âlikeâ a substance like coffee or beer the first time we drink it, but many of us come to like it. All tastes are, in essence, âacquired tastes.â Or, as Pelchat suggests, âan acquired
liking
is really what we should say.â
And when we talk about âacquired tastes,â we should really be talking about âacquired flavors,â as Dana Small, an associate fellow in Yale Universityâs John B. Pierce Laboratory who studies the neuropsychology of eating, suggested to me. We are not born knowing about flavors like coffee; we simply know the drink as bitter and thus bad. âThe bitter is there as a sign that there is a potential toxin in whatever youâre sampling,â she said. âYou just want to know that; you donât want to have to learn that.â
But no one is born liking, or not liking, chicken feet. Those âgatekeeperâ taste systems, after all, would not likely know feet from wing. It is all chicken. Before food even gets to us, culture has done that first big sort, sifting out the boundaries of what is acceptable to like. âThe French eat horses and frogs but the British eat neither,â notes Jared Diamond.As with any food, the French, during a discrete historical moment, had to be taught to âlearn to likeâ horse as food. But what we like in
taste
, as opposed to flavor, is remarkably similar around the world. As John Prescott writes in
Taste Matters
, âThe sweet taste of sucrose in water, is optimally pleasant at around 10â12 percent by weight (approximately the same as is found in many ripe fruits), regardless of whether you are from Japan, Taiwan, or Australia.â
Flavor conditioning helps us to like or dislike flavors. The benefit of this is, as Small put it, that we can âlearn to like the foods that are available to us, and avoid particular foods rather than entire classes of nutrients.â When she was young, she went to a popular sailing event in her hometown of Victoria, British Columbia. With college friends, she partook of one too many drinks of Malibu and 7UP, an unholy andintensely cloying concoction of sweet, coconut-flavored rum and citrusy soda pop. âThat was twenty years ago,â she recalled. âI canât even wear coconut suntan lotion. It makes me ill.â
Through a complex chain of activity in the brain, she said, we learn âflavor objectsââthe âperceptual gestaltâ of touch, taste, and smell in everything we eat. âDid this food make me sick? Did this food give me energy? You learn preferences based on the entire flavor object.â The flavor object itself is âcreatedâ by a network of neural activity, described as âa distributed circuit including the neural representation of the odor object, unimodal taste cells, unimodal oral somatosensory cells, multimodal cells, and a âbinding mechanism.âââ You do not just âtasteâ a strawberry; you virtually conjure it into being.
Coffeeâthe actual substanceâbecomes no less bitter the hundredth time we drink it than the first time we drank it. But something happens. âIt
becomes
coffee,â Small said. âThe brain has learned that coffee is not a