to a handful of networks playing the same old shows.
But I have come to Pelchatâs office to talk about liking. Regardless of which part of my mouth and nasal cavities are telling me what the flavor is, what is telling me I like it? Virginia Woolf once wrote that âreading is a longer and more complicated process than seeing.â So too is the question of whether we like something more than feeling a sensory response to something we have put in our mouths.What welike is sometimes corrupted by what we know we like. A study that had consumers test pineapple varieties found those who preferred pineapples labeled âorganicâ and âfree tradeâ tended to be those who were more fond of organic and free trade produce itself. Those less keen on organics were less happy about the pineapple. As the researchers noted, âThe same cognitive information evoked opposite affective reactions in different subjects.â
Pelchat, it turned out, did have some tea for me. But first she wanted me to take a capsule, which would contain either sugar or, simply, non-caloric cellulose. She wanted to show me the taste mechanism known as âflavor-nutrientâ conditioningâthe idea that we like what makes us feel good,
even if we do not know it
.
The power of this conditioning has been shown in any number of studies on rats, our fellow neophobic omnivores. Typically, a rat will drink, ad libitum (as much as it wants), something like orange Kool-Aid. Rats, as a cursory glance at the scientific literature reveals, drink a
lot
of Kool-Aid. Meanwhile, sometime before, during, or after, a sweetener will be âinfused,â via âintragastric catheter,â directly to its stomach. Later, the rat will sample grape Kool-Aid without getting the sugar drip to the stomach. When both flavors are later tested, rats will prefer the one that
was
sweetened, even when both flavors are now unsweetened. Sometimes they still cling to the old favorite when one of the
new
options actually tastes sweet in the moment.
Curiously, the way the rat came to like one flavor over another had nothing to do with a taste preference. How do researchers know? âIn fact,â Pelchat tells me, her voice lowering a bit, âthe esophagus is externalized.â With the gullet sitting outside its body, the rat cannot taste the glucose, nor could he belch it back up into his mouth. Infused into the stomach, however, that sweetness still provides a hedonic payoff. âSomething in the gut or the metabolic system is making them like that flavor,â Pelchat said.
Pelchat wondered if humansâ sensory mechanisms could be similarly bypassed, without such extreme surgery. So she once swallowed a nasogastric tube for a day and tried to mainline glucose. âI thought, I know what Iâm doing, Iâll pretend itâs food and Iâll swallow it, itâll be fine. Instead, I was puking, tearing up.â Finally, she hit upon pills, which would or would not release sweetness into the gut. A placebo cellulose pill has no calories, no benefit for the body. Well, almost nobenefit. âIncidentally,â she noted with a laugh as I inspected a pill, âthat will keep you regular.âIn her study, people who downed the (tasteless) sugar pills ended up liking the flavor of tea more than the tea they drank with the unsweetened pills.
So without even knowing why, people preferred one tea over another (
we are strangers to our taste
). They were getting âpost-ingestiveâ signals, in the form of a nutritional reward, that predisposed them toward a flavor. âI always make a point of telling people that reward and pleasure are not the same thing,â she says. âFood can be rewarding without the conscious experience of pleasure.â How we have all known this, eating in front of the television. The reverse can happen as well. Cancer patients who sampled a novel ice cream flavor prior to chemotherapy,