theoryâwhich are addressed in the last section of this chapter. I am not aware of any serious arguments for competitionâs inevitability explicitly offered from within these fields, however.
20. David W. and Roger T. Johnson, âInstructional Goal Structure: Cooperative, Competitive, or Individualisticâ (hereafter âStructureâ), p. 218.
21. Ashley Montagu cited in Johnson and Johnson, âCooperation in Learning: Ignored But Powerful,â p. 1. Psychiatrist Roderic Gorney agrees: âAny objective appraisal of modern man will disclose that in the overwhelming preponderance of human interactions cooperation
completely overshadows
competitionâ (
Human Agenda,
pp. 101â2; his emphasis). See also Arthur W. Combs,
Myths in Education,
pp. 15â17.
22. âAggression, anxiety, guilt, and self-centered motives and behavior have been so much the cloth of theory and research that questions of a âsofterâ side of young human beings seem almost unscientificâ (Marian Radke Yarrow et al., âLearning Concern for Others,â p. 240).
23. For example, see H. L. Rheingold and D. F. Hay, âProsocial Behavior of the Very Youngâ; Marian Radke Yarrow and Carolyn Zahn Waxier, âThe Emergence and Functions of Prosocial Behaviors in Young Childrenâ; Yarrow et al., âLearning Concernâ; James H. Bryan, âProsocial Behaviorâ; Maya Pines, âGood Samaritans at Age Two?â; and my own âThat Loving FeelingâWhen Does It Begin?â Yarrow and Waxier write as follows: âThe capabilities for compassion, for various kinds of reaching out to others in a giving sense are viable and effective responses early in life. . . . Very young children were often finely discriminative and responsive to othersâ need statesâ (pp. 78â79).
24. Rheingold and Hay, p. 101. On the other hand, Roderic Gorney claims that the view âthat human beings are predisposed to be not individualistic and competitive but social and cooperative . . . [is] endorsed by the majority of students of human social biology and evolutionâ
(Human Agenda,
p. 140). Also see Martin L. Hoffman, âIs Altruism Part of Human Nature?â
25. Richard Hofstadter,
Social Darwinism in American Thought,
p. 204.
26. Stephen Jay Gould, personal communication, 1984.
27. Charles Darwin,
The Origin of Species,
Chapter III, p. 62. Thus, Patrick Bateson writes that âthe restriction of differential survival to mean simply conflict is an obvious abuse of Darwinâs thinkingâ (âCooperation and Competition,â p. 55).
28. George Gaylord Simpson,
The Meaning of Evolution,
p. 222. Similarly, Ashley Montagu writes: âNatural selection . . . has been rather more operative in terms of co-operation than it has been in terms of what is generally understood by competition. . . . Natural selection through âcompetitionâ may secure the immediate survival of certain types of âcompetitors,â but the survivors would not long survive if they did not co-operateâ
(Darwin, Competition and Cooperation,
pp. 70, 72).
29. Petr Kropotkin,
Mutual Aid,
pp. 74â75; emphasis in original.
30. A revised version of W. C. Alleeâs
The Social Life of Animals
.
31. Montagu lists some fifty books and articles in
Darwin,
pp. 35â37.
32. Marvin Bates,
The Nature of Natural History
, quoted in Montagu, ibid., p. 58. William Pattenâs better known
The Grand Strategy of Evolution
summarized it as follows: âThere is but one creative process common to all phases of evolution, inorganic, organic, mental, and social. That process is best described by the term cooperation, or mutual serviceâ (p. 33). See also Roderic Gorneyâs
Human Agenda,
esp. pp. 100â101, and Lewis Thomasâs
The Lives of a Cell
. Even on the genetic level, Patrick Bateson points out that âthe perpetuation of each gene